Saturday 30 June 2012

The Listening Skill: Cognitive Processes, the L2 Classroom, and an Authentic Listening Text



The Listening Skill:  Cognitive Processes, the L2 Classroom, and an Authentic Listening Text
Christopher Stern




Listening to speech in order to construct meaning is an intricate process, which, because we engage in it on a daily basis in order to function as speakers of our native language, we generally overlook.  In this paper, I will first explore the cognitive processes and other mental features of listening in our native language, and then examine how they can shed light on the activity of listening in an L2, and help to inform more effective practice of listening to the L2 as the acquisition of a skill.  Finally, I will review a listening text (Real Lives, Real Listening, Unit 1) in order to critically examine the tasks and text included in consideration of those cognitive processes and mental features, and the degree to which they are activated, overlooked or further developed.

The Cognitive Process and Mental Features of Listening
In examining the cognitive process of listening to speech (herein: listening), it may be most central to introduce the concepts of bottom-up and top-down processing, both of which play an important part in the creation of meaning.  Stated simply, bottom-up processing is processing beginning at the phonemic level – putting sounds together to form words, words to form phrases, phrases to form sentences.  Top-down processing occurs though relating information from bottom-up processing to any relevant information we already have stored in our long-term memory.  From our long-term memory, in top-down processing we access “three types of background knowledge … (1) linguistic information … (2) knowledge of the world … (3) knowledge of discourse structure” (Omaggio, 2001: 145)
The cooperative and co-dependent bottom-up and top-down processes are just the starting point in modeling the cognitive process of listening.  The model for this process comprises three distinct mental entities – the memory store, working (or short-term) memory, and long-term memory.  A dedicated memory store exists for each of the five senses, for the purpose of retaining, for a very short period, sensory information in an unanalyzed form.  There are three stages to the cognitive process of listening, calling on both bottom-up and top down processing.  In the first stage, sounds come into the auditory sensory store.  This is the basis for bottom-up processing.  It has been found that auditory information can be retained in its unanalyzed form for up to four seconds in the sensory store.  In the second stage, this auditory information is held in the working memory while it is checked for concordances against information in the long-term memory in order to construe meaning.  This is the activity of top-down processing.  In the third stage, once meaning has been created, it may be transferred from working to long-term memory.  It is instructive to note that such information is kept in a reduced form. (Underwood, 1989: 2)
                Carroll, in Psychology of Language, makes three very useful observations related to the mental process of listening.  First, he states “The relatively longer duration of the auditory store may enable us to reanalyze auditory messages that we did not understand initially.”  (Carroll, 1999: 48)  Secondly, he notes that most information in the sensory stores “disappears very rapidly because it is not germane to our current goals.” (Carroll, 1999: 48)  And finally, he observes that in order to store information more efficiently we “chunk the words into grammatical constituents such as noun and verb phrases” (Carroll, 1999: 51). 
                Of additional use in understanding the process of listening is the concept of automatic and controlled processes.  Carroll notes that the working memory has a limited processing capacity (Carroll, 1999: 49), and defines controlled processes as those which draw upon this limited capacity.  He notes the importance of this concept when considering the cognitive load of complex tasks, and the possibility of cognitive overload leading to “impaired performance.” In contrast, automatic processes are those which “do not require extensive capacity.”  (Carroll 1999: 54)  In regards to listening, “One language processing task that is automatic, at least for adults, is recognizing common words.”  (Carroll 1999: 55)
Given the importance of a goal in the cognitive process of listening, it is appropriate to consider the reasons for which listening takes place in authentic situations.  These have been well delineated by Underwood.  He summarizes by providing “five main reasons for listening …(a) to engage in social rituals (b) to exchange information (c) to exert control (d) to share feelings (e) to enjoy yourself”  (Underwood, 1989: 4)  He then goes on to delineate all kinds of listening situations for which students should be prepared.  Although perhaps outdated in regards to genre, these include all the typical situations in which one listens to one’s native language.  More pertinent than listing the various situations in which native speakers listen, is to note the various goal-dependent roles of the listener.  These include listener as information gatherer (as in when listening to a lecture), listener as audience (watching TV or a film, listening to radio), listener as participant (and speaker, co-creating and directing the event), as side participant, and as overhearer.  (Flowerdew, 2005:89)  In each case, the listener has a different purpose, by which what is germane and what is not is determined.  It is instructive to note that the last role, that of overhearer, while being  the most typical role of the student listener, is the least common role of the authentic listener, and the one which provides the fewest authentic listening goals.
Likewise, given the importance of top-down processing in the act of listening, it is instructive to examine all the elements which may come into play.  Flowerdew notes eight “distinct dimensions of listening …Individualized, Affective, Cross-cultural, Strategic, Social, Intertextual, Contextualized, Critical” (Flowerdew, 2005: 85).  While fitting well with Omaggio’s triadic framework of linguistic, world and discourse knowledge, all of these comprise a listener’s background knowledge or lack thereof, thus determining top-down processing capability.

Implications for Effective Listening Practice in the L2 Classroom
                Because of the dyadic nature of listening, it is important that listening tasks address both top-down and bottom-up processes.  While the bottom-up process only occurs through listening itself, the top-down process can be activated prior to listening, by providing contextual cues, notably within the realms of students’ linguistic and world knowledge.  This is known as activating the schemata.  Numerous authors concur on the importance of activating schemata, as well as on the importance of choosing a text for which students have a schema to be activated.  Omaggio states:  “learning must be meaningful to be effective and permanent.  For material to be meaningful, it must be clearly relatable to existing knowledge that the learner already possesses.”  (Omaggio, 2001: 144)  Furthermore, “comprehension …is not a matter of simply processing the words of the message, but involves fitting the meaning of the message to the schema that one has in mind.”  (Omaggio, 2001: 148)  This process of activating schemata can be accomplished in the classroom through introduction of any one of a number of facets of the listening – the topic, the relation of the speakers to each other, the location of the communication, the discourse genre, etc. – and asking students to consider what they already know in regards to it. 
Brown argues that “you don’t want to dwell too heavily on the bottom-up, for to do so may hamper the development of a learner’s all-important automaticity in processing speech.”  (Brown, 1994: 246)  While I agree with the importance of activating schemata, I take issue with his phrase “a learner’s all-important automaticity.”  Carroll notes that “not all top-down processing is facilitative …expectations may actually interfere with learning new material.”  (Carroll, 1999: 54)  That is to say, automatic reliance on top-down processing can lead to expectations which, when contradicted, are simply not perceived.
                Because, as Carroll noted, information that is not germane to our goals for listening quickly disappears from the sensory store, it is important to provide students goals in listening.  To some extent, goal generation occurs simply through the process of activating schemata.  But this in and of itself is not enough.  This is why pre-listening activities follow on with listening tasks, which are to be introduced and considered by the student before the listening itself, in order to provide the student a reason to listen.  Thus, prior to listening, activities are designed to address only two purposes:  providing context, and creating motivation.  (Field, 2002: 243) 
                Finally, in consideration of the limited processing capacity of working memory, it is important that listening tasks are presented in a way that is manageable for students.  Nunan, among many other authors, emphasizes the role of comprehensible input (Nunan, 2002:  238).  When using authentic texts, comprehensibility is best achieved by simplifying student tasks.  (Field, 2002: 244) Thus, through multiple listenings, tasks can be given which develop from an extensive, or gist-oriented, to an intensive, or detail-oriented nature.  Because auditory information is stored in a chunked or reduced form, questions requiring listening for detail should be given attention before listening, as a cue for what to listen out for, while summarizing activities may be information-specific but not word-specific.

Real Lives, Real Listening, Unit 1
The text and accompanying exercises chosen were taken from Real Lives, Real Listening, available at http://www.northstarelt.co.uk/, and billed as a “new series of ready-made, unscripted, authentic listening materials featuring native and non-native speakers.”  As can be seen from the transcript, this particular listening takes the form of an interview, and, as the interviewer’s questions follow quite naturally on ‘Scott’s’ replies, seems quite likely to be truly authentic, rather than even semi-scripted.  In order to direct discussion of the tasks, I will summarize the transcript as being an interview with Scott, a young Australian living in a suburb of London, regarding the various living situations and neighbourhoods he has lived in in London.
The first task of Section 1 (Pre-Listening Comprehension) is titled Schema building, and this seems a good start.  However, it takes the form of multiple choice questions, all relating to Australia (none of which are answered in the text), while none of the listening text relates to Australia in any way, and so, as an exercise in schema-building seems to miss the point.  Task B, Discussion, follows on with a further question regarding Australia, a question asking students to consider why Scott may have left, and finally asking whether students know what an Australian accent sounds like.  While the last question is unlikely to yield any discussion beyond “yes”,” no”, or “difficult to understand,” the first two, again do not serve to prepare the learners in any way for what they will hear.  A better schema building exercise, given the content of the text, may be to ask students to discuss what they know about London, what sort of living arrangements young unmarried people often have in Western countries, and the advantages and disadvantages of living in certain types of neighbourhoods.   The last task in this section, Normalisation, is a very good idea, as it “is designed to help you get used to Scott’s voice.”  However, it is nothing more than a gap-fill exercise.  As such, it is technically an intensive listening task rather than a pre-listening task.  While pedagogically it seems sound in encouraging students to examine the exercise before listening to try to guess the (grammatical) types of words that may be needed, practically it is over-challenging as a listening activity, as all the information comes in far too quickly – most likely confusing students and interrupting their transfer from sensory store to paper.  Rather than ‘normalise’ students to Scott’s voice it may simply serve to convince them of the difficulty of catching so much information in so short a time span.
In Section 2 (Listening Comprehension), we see a continuation of intensive listening tasks, where students are asked to listen for very specific information.  The tasks take the form of questions which must be answered with specific, discrete information (A, C and E), or gap-fill (B, D and F). Furthermore, at no point are students encouraged to discuss or check their answers with other students.   Ideally, at this point, students should first be given a more global listening task, for example:  Does Scott live in a nice part of London?  Why do you think he does or doesn’t?  Discuss your answer with your friends.  After such a task, students then can move on to listening a second time for more specific information. 
Section 3, titled Interesting Language Points, begins with an examination of contrasting uses of simple and continuous verb forms in tasks A and B.   As this is grammar- and not listening-specific it is not germane to our discussion.  However, tasks C and D proceed to examine aspects of speech particular to the Australian accent.  The following Section 4, Further Listening Practice, is of more interest from a listening perspective.  Task A, Recognising sentence stress, would prove useful to listeners, with the added bonus that it asks students (unfortunately, again singly rather than in pairs), to predict which words might be stressed, then to check their predictions against the listening.  Tasks B, C and E deal with minimal pairs, and task D with linking.  In training students’ bottom-up listening skills these are sound (no pun intended) exercises.  Unfortunately, this section of tasks fails to recognize the most interesting language points in regard to authentic spoken English – the ‘Um’s, the pauses, the reformulations, the back-channeling on the part of the interviewer, the ‘Yeah, yeah’ and collocated chunks of language such as “It’s all about …”
While there are an additional four pages of tasks, I will finish my analysis here, with the brief note that as opposed to the further gap-fill exercises, it would probably be much more instructive to focus in post-listening on class-specific difficulties that come up during the course of the initial listenings.  As Field has stated:  “the main aim of a listening lesson is diagnostic …A diagnostic aim for the listening lesson implies a change in lesson shape …it is much more fruitful to allow time for an extended post-listening period in which learners’ problems can be identified and tackled.” (Field, 2002: 246)
                In summary, while providing students exposure to authentic listening texts is a necessity in the classroom, this is only one element of a well-thought out listening lesson.  A well-developed listening lesson will take into consideration all aspects of the actual cognitive processes of listening, encouraging both top-down and bottom up processing, activating relevant schemata, as well as conversation, examining aspects specific to spoken language, and allowing time to deal with class-specific issues as they arise.





















Sources:


Brown, D.  (1994).  Teaching by Principles.  Location:  Prentice Hall.
Carroll, D. (1999).  Psychololgy of Language.  Location:  Brooks/Cole.
Field, J. in Richards & Renandya, Eds.  (2002).  Methodology in Language Teaching.  Cambridge:  CUP.
Flowerdew, J. & Miller, L.  (2005).  Second Language Listening.  New York:  CUP.
Lam, W. in Richards & Renandya, Eds.  (2002).  Methodology in Language Teaching.  Cambridge:  CUP.
Nunan, D. (1991).  Language Teaching Methodology.  Location:  Prentice Hall.
Nunan, D. in Richards & Renandya, Eds.  (2002).  Methodology in Language Teaching.  Cambridge:  CUP.
Omaggio Hadley, A.  (2001).  Teaching Language in Context, 3rd Ed.  Boston:  Heinle & Heinle.
Richards & Renandya, Eds.  (2002).  Methodology in Language Teaching.  Cambridge:  CUP.
Underwood, M.  (1989). Teaching Listening.  Location:  Longman.



REAL LIVES, REAL LISTENING series
Real lives coverYOUNG ADULT/ADULT
Author: SHEILA THORN
Levels: Elementary, Intermediate & Advanced
A new series of ready-made, unscripted, authentic listening materials featuring native and non-native speakers. Created by Sheila Thorn of The Listening Business these materials train, rather than just test, the students in listening. They boost students’ confidence in their listening skills by exposing them to authentic texts, and introduce the learner to the grammatical structures and lexis which are used most frequently in spoken English. The materials also help students deal with those aspects of informal spoken English which they find so challenging, e.g. assimilation, elision, linking and colloquial expressions.
Each topic in the series is at three levels (Elementary, Intermediate and Advanced) and each book includes a free CD with five interviews (different interviews at every level), each followed by detailed comprehension and language practice exercises which can be used in the classroom or for self-study.
The series reflects the latest academic theories on the importance of authentic listening practice in language acquisition. The series also shows our new awareness of the huge differences between spoken and written English highlighted by recent research on spoken English corpora. Forthcoming titles are listed below, and the series will feature over 100 native and near-fluent non-native English speakers from all the main regions of Britain, as well as Australia, Canada, the USA, South Africa, Africa, China, Czech Republic, France, Germany, Greece, Italy, Japan, The Netherlands, Norway, Russia, South Korea, Sweden and Turkey.
The books come with a free CD, comprehensive transcripts and helpful glossaries. Teachers’ notes are free online, containing classroom support and background notes.
source:                 http://www.northstarelt.co.uk/

No comments:

Post a Comment