Monday 7 January 2013

CHAPTER 5 CONCLUSIONS, IMPLICATIONS AND DIRECTIONS FOR FURTHER STUDY





CHAPTER 5   
CONCLUSIONS, IMPLICATIONS
AND DIRECTIONS FOR FURTHER STUDY

Preface
            As Kathryn Schultz explains in her short essay The Pessimistic Meta-induction from the History of Science, “Because so many scientific theories from bygone eras have turned out to be wrong, we must assume that most of today’s theories will eventually prove incorrect as well.”  Knowledge creators realize “that they are part of a long process of approximation.  They know they are constructing models rather than revealing reality.”  (Schultz, K. in Brockman, J., ed., 2012, p. 30)  David Deutsch describes this phenomenon as “emergence.”  He states “The partial success of each theory in a sequence of improving theories is tantamount to the existence of a ‘layer’ of phenomena that each theory explains successfully – though, as it then turns out, partly mistakenly.”  (Deutsch, 2011, p. 111).  He continues to explain:  “With the hindsight provided by each successive theory, we can see not only where the previous theory made false predictions, but also that wherever it made true predictions this was because it had expressed some truth about reality.”  In consideration of this, any conclusions presented here are given not as absolute truths, but rather, come with the understanding that knowledge creation is process of gradual accumulation, and are to be viewed as intermediary steps on the path towards a more perfect understanding.
            Additionally, because the nature of ethnographic study is to produce “thick” detailed description of a single case study, and not broad, generalizable data, the conclusions drawn here relate only to this particular case study, although some of the patterns identified may have reach beyond its own confines.  As reach is defined as “The ability of some explanations to solve problems beyond those that they were created to solve” (Deutsch, 2011, p. 30), it may be the burden of future studies to demonstrate which of these patterns do have reach, and under what circumstances.

5.1 Conclusion
Within the tradition of ethnographic research, conclusions arise at three levels:  item level, pattern level, and structural level.  (LeCompte & Schensul, 1999, pp. 151 – 5)  Initial item level conclusions were reached early on in the literature review of this study, and served to provide the frameworks used for analyzing data.  Additional items emerged during the course of the research as the sum of individual Internet-mediated activities performed, both those reported in the ethnography, and by language learners through surveys and interviews.  At the pattern level, themes, typically those matching individual activities with the framework codes indicating (affective and learning) elements they comprise, arise.  An example would be the observation that blogging offers opportunity for L2 output, while also generating agency in the sense in which Gee uses the word, as ‘a real sense of ownership over what they are doing.’  Finally, the structural level of analysis provides “the entire picture.”  While the first level of analysis has been well explored in Chapter Four, Results and Discussion, the latter two levels of analysis point towards and are therefore detailed in the following section 5.2, Implications for Learning.  First, however, let us re-examine our research questions:
Based upon all aspects of the research performed, it is clear that those activities which most enable or elicit the experience of agency depend first and foremost on the individual learner.  Considering that agency is intertwined with volition, each learner has their own personal set of priorities, likes, and dislikes.  Therefore, I can only speak to those activities which enabled agency for me, while eliciting play and positive affect, and of those reported by other learners.  For myself, activities which came to the forefront were:  chatting, writing blog posts, pursuing directed activities, and engaging with videos.  Those which did not match up were games, virtual environments, and listening and reading activities.  With the exception of writing blog posts, I found the same was true for the majority of learners of both English and Thai.  And, for all learners, without exception, chatting online in one’s second language stands out as the single most activity to generate agency while engaging learners in a meaning-focused flow experience. Because of the centrality of the social element in chatting, it is also the one activity in which learners were most likely to feel themselves at play rather than at work, and therefore to consider themselves at the time as people, rather than as learners or students. Because of the affective element of play within chatting, I would tend to agree with Klopfer, Osterwell and Salen (2009) with regard to their broad definition of play as an attitude towards one’s activity, rather than as a specific activity in and of itself.  Another activity that stands out, to a lesser degree, as central to many learners is that of using video online. 
However, many individual learners, of both Thai and English, had their own personal preferences, some even bordering on mania, for specific online activities.  For EK, these activities involved engaging in fandoms, watching series programmes, and writing joint fiction. TK displayed a passion for activities provided on directed learning sites such as its4thai.com and english2thai.com.  EC highlighted his experience seeking cultural knowledge through sites such as that hosted by the British Council.
With regard to insights into the mechanisms of the technology and the user experience of such technologies, the first pertinent observation is that the vast majority of learners, regardless of L2, tend to use multiple Internet-based platforms or functions simultaneously.  Another common theme was that of general complaint regarding the shortcomings of translation sites, and the relative lack of opportunity to practice both listening and speaking online.  Through my own experience, I also found that those sites offering listening activities lacking in technical options, such as variable playback speed and scroll-over translation, by which they would be greatly enhanced. Finally, nearly all learners reported utilizing sites for directed language learning, most of which provide ‘game-like’ activities or exercises.  Based upon my personal experience with such sites, those aspects which do enhance the affective experience of activities follow many of the qualities or mechanisms outlined by McGonigal, namely:  discrete, achievable tasks, fun failure, sufficient and immediate feedback, and a sense of ‘leveling up.’  Additionally, I noticed the propulsive effect of activities which have a kind of automaticity, allowing the learner to focus immediately on the correct solution or answer, without having to consider the how or why of what they are doing.
2. Secondly, does this emic perspective differ depending on the specific L2 being acquired through non-instructed, Internet-mediated L2 activity, and do similarities also occur?  The two L2 being examined here are Thai and English.  The link between these two questions lies in the conjecture that the L2 acquisition process, regardless of the target L2, basically proceeds in a similar fashion. 
Because of multiple confounding factors, such as age, culture, and others, any statement concerning differences between Thai and English L2 learners quickly becomes muddled and inconclusive.  Nonetheless, based on the survey and interview results, I can state that similarities in their experience of, and affective reaction to various Internet mediated L2 activities override differences.
To begin with background differences which may serve as confounding factors, data revealed that TLLs generally were older and had fewer years of L2 study.  While they used the Internet more frequently, they spent a smaller proportion of their online time, and did a narrower variety of activities, in the L2, than ELLs. 
TLLs generally enjoyed the same online activities in their L2 as ELLs, but more so.  However, they also expressed greater frustration or difficulty in using the Internet in their L2 than did ELLs.  This frustration was noted particularly with regard to level of language difficulty, especially in reading oriented activities.  Perhaps because of relative lack of exposure and familiarity, many TLLs, myself included, expressed a high degree of frustration with sites entirely in Thai, created for Thai users, than did ELLs for sites entirely in English, created for native English users.  Additionally, some TLLs commented on a relative lack of interest in Thai language content (perhaps as a result of its cultural specificity).  In contrast, ELLs expressed enthusiasm for the greater amount of useful information available in English online.  However, they also reported uncertainty regarding the correctness of English which they might encounter online, as so much content is produced by non-native speakers (as well as non-professional native speaker writers with a tenuous, at best, grasp of English grammar, sentence structure, and spelling). And finally, many ELLs commented enthusiastically on the ability the Internet offers them to chat with friends, both native and non-native speakers, worldwide.  As Thai is a national rather than international language, this option was not featured in any comments by TLLs.
3. Does this conjecture also hold true for L2 acquisition in an Internet-mediated environment?  If some differences do exist, these can then be seen as areas in which any conclusions regarding Thai L2 acquisition may not be transferrable to English L2 acquisition, and vice versa.  It may also be interesting to consider whether such possible differences result from differences in the target L2, or differences in the cultural and technical knowledge backgrounds of the participants.  If similarities exist in some areas, these may be areas in which any observations or conclusions are transferable and applicable to any L2[H3] .[H4] 
While it has long been a topic of debate as to whether the L2 acquisition process is primarily the same or different depending on the target L2, this question has been broken down into a number of sub-factors.  These include the learner’s motivational profile, the learning environment, previous other L2 study, and the degree of difference between the learner’s native language and target language.  With specific regard to differences between English and Thai as target L2s, the issues of learners’ identities and language functionality also come into play, as English is a global language, and Thai is not.  For this same reason, it is evident that there are indeed significant differences in the learning environments accessible through the Internet to learners of either language.  As Thai serves as a national language only, it presents the thoughts, concerns, themes, and collective knowledge of the Thai nation and people only.  English, on the other hand, as a global and international language, is the default language of the Internet, with the vast bulk of content (more that 80% of Internet home pages are in English) (http://englishenglish.com/english_facts_8.htm) available on the Internet is available in English.  Information from Wikipedia indicates that:  “Estimates of the percentages of Web sites using various content languages as of 30 December 2011:  English - 56.6%, Thai – 0.3%.” 
Nonetheless, the results of this study appear to indicate that with regard to the learning and affective mechanisms of online L2 activity, similarities outweigh differences.  Any differences that do exist are minor, and relate more to differences in the online presence of the two languages, and differences in the cultural and technical knowledge backgrounds of the participants, rather than differences in the actual second language acquisition process.

5.2  Implications for Online Learning
            Based on the data accumulated, we can summarize the types of learning as well as the predominant affective reactions to the main types of online activity reviewed in this study.            Chatting offers multiple benefits by engendering opportunities for meaning focused input, production, and interaction.  In comparison to live conversation, for which it serves as a parallel activity, it allows participants freedom of effort, in being able to respond when one likes, and also use of JIT and on-demand functions, which may partially explain reduced anxiety levels.   Input and feedback from interlocutors can offer participants a chance to focus on form, and to practice and learn set phrases, and the type of vocabulary to which participants are exposed is usually more colloquial than that found in written texts.  Chatting can also serve as scaffolding for live conversation.
            Reading, obviously, provides language input.  Based on the data, the importance of material that is appropriate to the learner’s level is clear.  This is a concern which may not always be effectively addressed by unaided learners, and offers an opportunity for teacher input.  Another concern, particularly for online readers, is the assured correctness of any chosen text, again, an area for teacher input.  Finally, what emerged as crucial in the selection of online reading material is the element of personal interest.  Also, from my own personal experience with vocabulary and grammar focused websites with no accompanying text, and online texts with no accompanying grammar or vocabulary focus, any reading passage should ideally be integrated with a relevant vocabulary and grammar focus.
            Online listening activities are usually, and ideally, accompanied by a transcript and / or occur within a contextualizing video.  As with reading passages, they obviously provide input, and the importance of level-appropriate material is clear.  With regard to technical features, most online listening activities could be greatly enhanced by a few, simple changes.  Variable speed playback allows learners to ramp up ability; text which changes color as it occurs audibly (as with karaoke text) allows learners to keep track of where they are; and scroll over features could allow learners JIT access to meanings and individual word pronunciation.  Some of these technical features would serve well to enhance reading focused online activity as well.
            Writing online, here subsumed under the heading of blogging, obviously provides opportunity for output.  It also allows participants opportunities for developing a personal voice, and the creation of and investment in one or many online identities.  More than any other online activity, it allows learners to exercise agency as ownership over what they are doing.  It also allows participants to customize their activity, opportunity to contextualize lexis, and to create their own scaffolding and system thinking. The type of vocabulary acquired through blogging can vary widely, but is likely to be of personal relevance to the learner.  Finally, the importance of finishing what one has started should not be underestimated.
            Directed sites for the learning of particular second languages seem to hold some attraction for all learners.  They frequently offer ‘game-like’ aspects, which make activities more motivating and fun:  discrete, achievable tasks, fun failure, automaticity, feedback, and leveling up.   The type of vocabulary usually acquired through such sites is primarily functional.  Finally, as with games, which I do not focus on here due to their relative lack of popularity within this study, online activity should appropriately match a learner’s profile.  While many learners prefer play to study, and are therefore attracted to games and game-like activity, some are not.  For more ‘serious’ learners, such activity may not be motivating or appropriate. 
            With video, as with reading, the importance of a learner’s personal interest in the subject matter is paramount.  Video is the most accessible of all online activities in that it can be an almost entirely passive experience, requiring the learner to simply choose, click and consume.  However, in order to learn from videos, the learner must develop a methodology.  Ultimately, watching video provides listening practice for learners.  Therefore, the methodology is not dissimilar to that required for listening activities:  the application of effort to identify and learn unknown vocabulary, the need for repeated listening / viewing, and, ideally, the execution of some sort of comprehension checking activity, which might comprise a set of teacher designed questions, or alternately some sort of learner generated, text, audio, or video based response.   


5.2.1  Implications for Autonomous Learning
There is a direct positive relation between agency and autonomy, and between autonomy and motivation.  Nevertheless, autonomy must be nurtured and coached.  Learners often are accustomed to doing what they are told to do, and such learners may have a less well-developed sense of their autonomy and how to cultivate it.  Therefore, in such cases, a process of re-learning to do what one wants to do (agency) must be consciously undertaken.  Paradoxically, it is the role of the teacher to re-introduce the concept of autonomy and help learners cultivate it. 
One theme which clearly emerged from the data is the importance of personal interest and significance and their direct positive relation to autonomy, hence motivation.  An obvious starting point for teachers and learners in search of autonomy is the exploration and cultivation of personal interests, and the consideration of areas of personal significance to learners. 
Given the centrality of chatting in this study, in data coming both from the ethnography and the surveys and interviews, it would appear that both agency and autonomy are closely related to community and relationship building.  EK’s experience with fandoms and writing fan fiction serves as an excellent example of how involvement in a community of like-minded people expands and productively channels agency, and vice versa.  For this reason, encouraging learners to collaborate and build relationships with others who share similar interests should serve well to foster further autonomy and language learning.
Furthermore, autonomy, as well as motivation, can be greatly enhanced by appropriate feedback.  This was something I did not receive in my blogging experience, but which I did encounter through both chatting and sties for directed language study.  its4thai.com. featured advancing progress bars, while many of the activities on thai-language.com provided point scores indicating the percentage of correctly answered items.  These features served to propel my activity, to inform me of where I needed to focus, and to constantly challenge myself to improve my performance.
Lastly, although I began this study with only one understanding, or definition, of agency, I close it with two.  While agency can be conceived as a sixth freedom – to do what one wants, when one wants, it can also be conceived (in line with Gee’s understanding) as a sense of ownership over what one does.  This second understanding refers not only to the activity itself, but also to the product of that activity. 
Because of this, we can see a murky tripartite divide among activities, between those that are (primarily) consumptive, interactive, and productive, each offering greater degrees of Gee’s sense of agency.  Of course, there is a great deal of overlap with many activities, for example, the interactive nature of writing joint fan fiction is also productive, and relies as well on the consumptive aspect of viewing or reading certain existing genres.  It is also clear that there is an obvious order to types of activity, one which mirrors language acquisition, of consumptive to interactive to productive.  But, because of the additional sense of agency imparted by production, it may be sensible to emphasize productive activities over all others. 
Nonetheless, it is also clear that learner autonomy is equally achievable in any of these three categories of activity.  When a learner selects a video for consumption they are exercising autonomy to a degree equal to that in which they are writing a blog post.  The crux of autonomy here is the exercise of free choice.  In the interest of fostering greater autonomy through online second language activity, it is this aspect of choice that the teacher can help to foster.  When learners’ understanding of choice is informed by a greater awareness of multiple factors – their own ability levels, their own areas of interest, the range of options available to them, and some possible methods for exploring and exploiting those options – then they achieve greater autonomy in their learning.

5.3 Directions for further study
            In this study, the focus has been on learners’ emic perspective towards their online second language activities, including their affective response to those activities, as well as the learning implications for those activities.  While the focus has been on learners’ self-directed activities, study of more structured, including instructed, online L2 activity is also warranted.  A number of other areas of interest have been touched upon in this study, but deserve to be researched in greater depth. 
Much of the emphasis in this study has been on the use of CMC, or chatting.  Again, the focus has been on the affective and learning aspects therein.  But further study, focusing specifically on relationship building, the nature of various online relationships, and their effect on the second language acquisition process, would be valuable.  Related to this, online communities and the process of acquisition of their specific linguistic norms is an area for further research.  Also related is the need for further research into the effects of inter-learner play, the possibility of transcendence of referential, or social, identity during such experiences, and ensuing implications for learning. 
While much has been written about the desirability of positive affect during the learning experience, additional study of the impact of the affective element on learning is needed.  In future it is quite likely that further exploration of this issue will be conducted in conjunction with emotions research, making use of state of the art tools such as brain imaging or MRI .
            Another area for further study is that of the leveraging of agency in online games and other activities.  While it has been postulated that the ability to eschew one’s physically based identity and create a new online persona can allow learners to interact in new, freer and less restrictive ways than they would in physical reality, research in this area is scant.  The possibilities exist that online activity can allow learners the opportunity to avoid the types of age, gender and race biases they might encounter in the ‘real’ world, but have yet to be proved.   
I have stated:  “In natural, uninstructed L2 acquisition it would appear that implicit knowledge would naturally occur, while explicit knowledge would require either instruction or metacognitive analysis on the part of the learner.”  Further research could also be directed to determine what kind of meta-cognitive analysis occurs in the learner in non-instructed learning which allows rule-based competency development. Such research could possibly make use of talk through protocol.
Finally, as this study is quite limited in scope, a broader study of what Internet-mediated experiences motivate learners may yield additional patterns as well as outliers. One outlier encountered in this study, that of fanfiction, indicates that others may follow, and that each particular outlier may open up new paths towards additional, highly specialized research.



“If we knew what we were doing, it wouldn’t be called research.”  (Einstein, A. as quoted by De Grey, A. in Brockman, J., ed., 2012, p. 55)